P O B O X 1 1 7 2 2 1 0 0 L U N D + 4 6 4 6 -2 2 2 0 0 0 0 Are All Opposites Equal -or Are Some More Equal than Others?
نویسنده
چکیده
for UCL, 27 November, 2013 Are all opposites equal – or are some more equal than others? Carita Paradis, Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University At the one extreme, antonyms show up as strongly associated pairs such as long–short, heavy–light, hot–cold and good–bad along the dimensions of LENGTH, WEIGHT, TEMPERATURE and MERIT, respectively, While other pairs appear to be less obviously or felicitously opposable and more clearly bound up with specific domains and situations, e.g. calm–high-strung, calm– flowing, calm–agitated, as in ‘I prefer calm dogs to high-strung dogs’, ‘I prefer calm waters to flowing waters’, ‘I prefer a calm public to an agitated public’. In spite of this difference, all of them are used to express binary opposition. In that sense they are all equal. But, what makes the former pairings more felicitously opposable than the latter ones still remains a mystery, at least in part. There are indications that it is the ‘goodness’ of the relations as such that is of importance, not lexical associations or co-occurrence frequency (van de Weijer, Paradis, Willners & Lindgren 2012). But, what then is this goodness? The purpose of this contribution is to try to determine why some pairs are felt to be “better” antonyms than others and therefore more apt to take on special status as canonical antonyms. What is the difference between pairs such as heavy–light and hot–cold on the one hand, and most other antonymic construals such as calm–high-strung or calm–flowing on the other? In order determine this we first need to explain how two expressions can be understood as antonyms, and for that we need a theoretical framework that is capable of accounting, not only for some couplings in language, but also for antonymic meaning creation in text and discourse. Couched in the framework of Lexical Meaning as Ontologies and Construals (Paradis 2005), this contribution treats antonymy as a spatial configuration construal grounded in perception and effected through comparison of the opposing properties. Whenever we think of something as ‘long’, ‘good’ or ‘dead’, it will be in contrast to something that lacks or has little of this property, i.e. their opposites. The proposal is that form–meaning pairings in language are antonyms when they are used as binary opposites in a given context. Characteristic of antonyms is that they share an important segment of meaning at the same time as they differ prominently along the same dimension. Configurationally, this translates into a spatial configuration construal where this simple content dimension, bounded (e.g. dead–alive) or unbounded (short–long), is divided in two parts by a BOUNDARY In contrast to a categorization by configuration, categorization by contentful meaning structures forms a continuum ranging from the strongly related pairings, referred to as canonical antonyms (e.g. long–short) to more peripheral members (e.g. calm–high-strung). In order to explain why some lexical semantic couplings tend to form conventionalized pairs, this proposal appeals to (i) their ontological set-up in terms of the simplicity, entrenchment and perceptual basicness of dimensions along which they evoke opposing properties, e.g. long–short of LENGTH as opposed to calm–high-strung of EMOTIONAL TENSION OF ANIMATE CREATURE, (ii) the configurational clarity and symmetry of the antonyms in relation to the BOUNDARY dividing the meaning structure, e.g. small–large is a better pair than small–huge because the properties are at the same distance from a middle-ground reference point (Paradis, Willners & Jones 2009, Bianchi, Savardi & Kubovy 2011, Paradis & Willners 2011, Jones, Murphy, Paradis & Willners 2012, van de Weijer, Paradis, Willners & Lindgren forthcoming). Data from a range of textual, behavioural and neurophysiological techniques are used to support the claims. Abstract for UCL, 27 November, 2013for UCL, 27 November, 2013 ReferencesBianchi, I., Savardi, U., & Kubovy, M. (2011). Dimensions and their poles: a metric andtopological approach to opposites. Language and cognitive processes, 26(8), 22–33.Jones, S., Murphy, M.L., Paradis, C. & Willners, C. (2012). Antonyms in English: Construals,constructions and canonicity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Paradis, C. (2005). Ontologies and construals in lexical semantics. Axiomathes 15. 541–573.Paradis, C., Willners, C. & Jones, S. (2009). Good and bad opposites: using textual andpsycholinguistic techniques to measure antonym canonicity. The Mental Lexicon 4(3).380–429.Paradis, C. & Willners, C. (2011). Antonymy: From convention to meaning-making. Reviewof Cognitive Linguistics 9(2). 367–391.van de Weijer, J., Paradis, C., Willners, C. & Lindgren M. (2012). As lexical as it gets: Therole of co-occurrence of antonyms in a visual lexical decision experiment. In DagmarDivjak & Stefan Th. Gries (eds.), Frequency effects in language representation, 255-279. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.van de Weijer, J., Paradis, C., Willners, C., & Lindgren, M. (forthcoming). Antonymcanonicity: Temporal and contextual manipulations. Brain and Language.
منابع مشابه
Online Analytical Processing Stream Data: Is It Feasible?
! " # %$& ' ( ! )+*, . ()&$/!0 , ' "# ! & %*,!& .!1 2*,3 !54 .! 6 7 % . ! ) *, '8 9:* ! $+ !&;0! =<> 1*, #* 3? % % @)0 6 &AB C * # ( */*D-1 4 # *FEG H " ! ! ) H 9 I ' JLK(*=MN . O6 H " *,3& % "P-#)0 6 B % ) Q L 6 *=MR P 1 . *,3& E % ". *,!SO M>% L *, % T ,!0 $/ % T % U ! % ) !V)&$&!0 "W" -0 ,! 4, +8 % "P-X 7 % ! ) D ! )Y*, & % <7 6 D % $ 4 -D . N*,3L ,E ". *,!SJ?ZL*H) " * 1 . N % "P-F4,-F P 1 ....
متن کاملSTUDENT PAPER: A Multiagent Reinforcement Learning Algorithm by Dynamically Merging Markov Decision Processes
! "$#% &#' &(*),+& &. . / 0#'1/ #'2 3 / 4 & ' 1/ +!)5. +6+ 78 %9 0#'1/:; = @#'1A ! B#C# !"%DB"E1/"C#'+ ' > "% FG !+6+BH?+& I+ 7J#'1A #'1/+! " #'+O#'2 P# &"%D Q32 !. 2R & 6# 1A" !. #'1/ & /+ !SUTV W#'2 1/" 7X &78 4Y Q= =),+& ' #'1/+! " #'+O @#'1/7 / P]W & 'D +&:^H . 1A"%1/+! 7J '+6. "%"% "?F_]^` a "[L ...
متن کاملEstimation of direct costs associated with cataracts in farmed Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)
A spreadsheet model that has been standardised for use across different livestock species and diseases, was utilised to estimate the direct costs of cataracts in farmed fish, using the Norwegian farmed salmon industry segment as an example. The data input for the model was obtained from previously published information and also from a small survey specifically carried out to obtain the addition...
متن کاملA Set of 74 Test Functions for Nonlinear Equation Solvers
This report gives 74 funct ions appropriate to use to test programs which solve one nonl inear equat ion in ofie real variable . The funct ions are given in a Fortran subrout ine as a two-dimensional array . A SET OF 74 TEST FUNCTIONS FOR WO* 'LINEAR F.OUATION SOLVERS INTRODUCTION AMP SUT1MA n Y: Tliis report «ives 74 functions in FORTRAN code which have been gathered as a set of test funct ion...
متن کاملApplied Mathematics Letters Finite Time Extinction in Nonlinear Diffusion Equations
K e y w o r d s F i n i t e travelling waves, Degenerate diffusion, Singular, Extinction. 1. I N T R O D U C T I O N This paper is concerned with the quasilinear parabolic equation _ m +l u, d (u ) ~ + f (u ) , (x, t) ~ Q := ]~ × (0, ~ ) , u(x, O) = Uo(Z), x ~ R, 0 <_ uo <_ 1, (1) (2) where m > 0, d = 1 / (m + 1), u0 E C(R), and f satisfies the following. (A1) f e C[0, 1] N CI(0, 1], f (0) = f ...
متن کاملCitrus Fruit Intake Substantially Reduces the Risk of Esophageal Cancer
Many epidemiologic studies indicate a potential association between fruit and vegetable intake and various cancers. The purpose of this meta-analysis is to investigate the association between citrus fruit intake and esophageal cancer risk. The authors conducted a comprehensive search on PubMed, EMBASE, and the Cochrane Library from inception until July 2014. Studies presenting information about...
متن کامل